NIH Calls for More (and Different) Research on Preventive Measures
Quick Links
A much greater investment in Alzheimer disease research will be needed before scientists can draw reliable conclusions about what factors increase or decrease a person’s risk of developing AD, and what interventions protect against the disease. This is the sobering conclusion an expert panel convened by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) made in an April 28 report and press release. The 15-member panel seated independent leaders drawn largely from outside the field of AD research itself. They examined the current evidence for a wide range of putative AD risk factors and interventions. Their verdict was not what the field, which of late has been abuzz with talk of prevention, hoped to hear. Although the panel found some consistent associations between certain risk and protective factors and AD, they judged that none of the data reached a level of scientific rigor and repeatability that would allow them to confidently make public health recommendations. This conclusion poured cold water on some much-touted associations, for example, for most dietary supplements. It also generated some controversy. To rectify the limitations of existing studies, the panel outlined an ambitious, multifaceted research agenda that includes calls for more large-scale longitudinal studies and for standardized measurements of cognitive function and risk factor exposure.
As previous reports have mentioned, the panel, drawn from the fields of gerontology, neurology, genetics, and nutrition, evaluated 25 reviews and 250 primary research studies, including both observational studies and randomized clinical trials (RCTs). To be considered, studies had to meet criteria that included a duration of one year or greater, 300 or more participants for observational studies, and 50 or more participants for RCTs. The reviewers evaluated nutritional factors, medical conditions, medications, social/economic/behavioral factors, environmental factors, and genetics. They used a grading system that rated the level of evidence for each finding as high, moderate, or low, with “low level of evidence” indicating that further research might change the estimate of a factor’s effect.
The reviewers found only a few factors that showed a consistent association with AD or cognitive decline. The strongest associations, for which the panel graded the level of evidence as moderate, showed an increased risk of AD associated with carrying the ApoE4 gene, and from taking conjugated equine estrogen combined with progesterone (not estrogen alone). Diabetes, depression, and current tobacco use were consistently associated with an increased risk of AD, while physical activity, a Mediterranean diet (low in saturated fat, high in grains, fruits, vegetables, nuts, fish, and olive oil), and high levels of cognitive engagement were consistently associated with a decreased risk of AD (e.g., Féart et al., 2009; Willis et al., 2006). For all of these factors, the panel graded the level of evidence as low. The panel found a high level of evidence that taking gingko biloba does not alter AD risk, and moderate evidence that vitamin E has no effect on AD risk.
A low level of evidence does not mean the studies are of poor quality, said Neil Buckholtz, who heads the Dementias of Aging Branch at the National Institute on Aging (NIA), but rather indicates that “we don’t yet have enough information to make specific recommendations.” These studies point to the most fertile areas to investigate, Buckholtz said. “We have to now firm up those data and those kinds of associations in further research.”
The panel identified several problems with the existing data. There are too few RCTs, considered the gold standard of research trials; many studies are too short, often lasting less than one year; and a lack of standardization makes comparison of outcomes across studies impossible. The panel urged the field to develop standardized, validated measures for risk factors and interventions that take into account the intensity, duration, and timing of exposure, as well as standardized methods for cognitive assessment. Because of the long prodromal phase of AD, during which no symptoms are seen, the panel recommended that exposure to risk factors be studied much earlier in life, years prior to the expected onset of symptoms, and that interventions should also be begun as early as possible.
It is too early to tell whether the panel’s report will prompt specific changes in NIA funding, but Buckholtz suggested that funding for epidemiological studies might become more of a priority. Some of the proposed changes are already in progress, Buckholtz said. The NIH Toolbox Project seeks to develop standardized measurements of cognitive function that could be used in future studies. The 29 NIA-funded Alzheimer’s Disease Centers collect uniform data, available from the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center, so that investigators can compare measures across centers. More reliable measurements of disease such as imaging and fluid biomarkers are being incorporated into more studies, thanks to projects such as the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI). Looking to the future, long-term, large-scale epidemiological studies similar to the Framingham Heart Study will be particularly important for collecting better data on AD risk and protective factors, Buckholtz said, but “these are big studies, they cost a lot of money, they go on for a long time. We try to fund as many of them as we can, but it’s difficult.”
Lack of funding is the main issue holding back progress, says the Alzheimer’s Association, which issued a statement supporting the NIH panel’s call for more research. “The research dollars allocated today don’t even come close to reflecting the actual public health need,” said the Association’s representative, Maria Carrillo. She pointed to the progress a sizable investment in research has made in other health areas, such as stroke and some cancers. “It’s time to really pay attention to Alzheimer disease research.” The Association is sponsoring the Alzheimer’s Breakthrough Act, which calls for $2 billion in AD funding, roughly a quadrupling of current research dollars. The bill was referred to the House Energy and Commerce Committee by its sponsor, Ed Markey, where it has languished since July 2009.
But the Alzheimer’s Association took issue with the NIH panel’s conclusion that current research is inadequate to make health recommendations. “The Association continues to stand by our brain health recommendations,” Carrillo said, which include staying physically, socially, and mentally active, and adopting a brain-healthy diet. “We do feel that some of the retrospective and epidemiological studies that have been done give us some good indications that brain health can make a difference in our future in terms of Alzheimer’s risk.”
For people concerned about AD risk, Buckholtz said, the panel’s report “is suggestive that there are some things you can do, in terms of a healthy diet, a healthy lifestyle.” He recommended physical exercise, social interactions, reducing high blood pressure and high cholesterol, and making lifestyle choices that reduce the risk of diabetes and cardiovascular disease. These lifestyle choices, Buckholtz said, “can certainly help maintain a healthy brain.”—Madolyn Bowman Rogers
Madolyn Bowman Rogers is a freelance writer in Madison, Wisconsin.
References
Paper Citations
- Féart C, Samieri C, Rondeau V, Amieva H, Portet F, Dartigues JF, Scarmeas N, Barberger-Gateau P. Adherence to a Mediterranean diet, cognitive decline, and risk of dementia. JAMA. 2009 Aug 12;302(6):638-48. PubMed.
- Willis SL, Tennstedt SL, Marsiske M, Ball K, Elias J, Koepke KM, Morris JN, Rebok GW, Unverzagt FW, Stoddard AM, Wright E, . Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. JAMA. 2006 Dec 20;296(23):2805-14. PubMed.
External Citations
Further Reading
Papers
- Daviglus ML, Bell CC, Berrettini W, Bowen PE, Connolly ES, Cox NJ, Dunbar-Jacob JM, Granieri EC, Hunt G, McGarry K, Patel D, Potosky AL, Sanders-Bush E, Silberberg D, Trevisan M. NIH State-of-the-Science Conference Statement: Preventing Alzheimer's Disease and Cognitive Decline. NIH Consens State Sci Statements. 2010 Apr 28;27(4) PubMed.
- Williams JW, Plassman BL, Burke J, Benjamin S. Preventing Alzheimer's disease and cognitive decline. Evid Rep Technol Assess (Full Rep). 2010 Apr;(193):1-727. PubMed.
- Daviglus ML, Plassman BL, Pirzada A, Bell CC, Bowen PE, Burke JR, Connolly ES, Dunbar-Jacob JM, Granieri EC, McGarry K, Patel D, Trevisan M, Williams JW. Risk factors and preventive interventions for Alzheimer disease: state of the science. Arch Neurol. 2011 Sep;68(9):1185-90. PubMed.
Primary Papers
- Daviglus ML, Bell CC, Berrettini W, Bowen PE, Connolly ES, Cox NJ, Dunbar-Jacob JM, Granieri EC, Hunt G, McGarry K, Patel D, Potosky AL, Sanders-Bush E, Silberberg D, Trevisan M. National Institutes of Health State-of-the-Science Conference statement: preventing alzheimer disease and cognitive decline. Ann Intern Med. 2010 Aug 3;153(3):176-81. PubMed.
Annotate
To make an annotation you must Login or Register.
Comments
College of Medicine, University of Arizona
I am delighted that research on prevention of Alzheimer disease is now a high priority for the NIH. This is particularly critical to the population at greatest lifetime risk for development of AD, postmenopausal women. One correction to this report is critical. The comment that "taking conjugated equine estrogen combined with progesterone (not estrogen alone)" is associated with AD or cognitive decline is not entirely accurate. Progesterone was not the progestin used in the WHIMS clinical trial which is likely to be the study from which the panel's conclusion was drawn. The progestin used in WHIMS was medroxyprogesterone acetate (MPA). The distinction is critical as MPA can completely antagonize the benefits of estrogen in brain, whereas progesterone can, in some instances, enhance estrogen action (1-3).
Moreover, recent data indicate that the regimen of progesterone exposure, cyclic vs. continuous, can drastically affect outcomes in brain that are particularly relevant to development of AD pathology. Christian Pike's team, as part of our NIA program project on progesterone, found that continuous progesterone blocked the Aβ-lowering action of estrogen. In contrast, cyclic progesterone significantly reduced β amyloid levels by itself and enhanced rather than inhibited the estrogen effects.
Collectively, these data and those that are emerging from our group indicate that clinically used progestins, such as progesterone and MPA, can have drastically different outcomes on brain responses relevant to AD and that the regimen of delivery of these molecules is also critically important.
References:
Brinton RD, Nilsen J. Effects of estrogen plus progestin on risk of dementia. JAMA. 2003 Oct 1;290(13):1706; author reply 1707-8. PubMed.
Nilsen J, Brinton RD. Divergent impact of progesterone and medroxyprogesterone acetate (Provera) on nuclear mitogen-activated protein kinase signaling. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003 Sep 2;100(18):10506-11. PubMed.
Brinton RD, Thompson RF, Foy MR, Baudry M, Wang J, Finch CE, Morgan TE, Pike CJ, Mack WJ, Stanczyk FZ, Nilsen J. Progesterone receptors: form and function in brain. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2008 May;29(2):313-39. PubMed.
Carroll JC, Rosario ER, Villamagna A, Pike CJ. Continuous and cyclic progesterone differentially interact with estradiol in the regulation of Alzheimer-like pathology in female 3xTransgenic-Alzheimer's disease mice. Endocrinology. 2010 Jun;151(6):2713-22. PubMed.
Solo practitioner and independent researcher; Founder, National Institute of Good Health
In my opinion, the big environmental cause of AD is likely to be refined, antioxidant-depleted seed oils, which have now been linked to both cognitive decline (Psaltopoulou et al., 2008) and incident AD (Barberger-Gateau et al., 2007). These oils are neurotoxic, and their absence in the diet of people using only olive oil accounts for the relative absence of AD in this group.
References:
Psaltopoulou T, Kyrozis A, Stathopoulos P, Trichopoulos D, Vassilopoulos D, Trichopoulou A. Diet, physical activity and cognitive impairment among elders: the EPIC-Greece cohort (European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition). Public Health Nutr. 2008 Oct;11(10):1054-62. PubMed.
Barberger-Gateau P, Raffaitin C, Letenneur L, Berr C, Tzourio C, Dartigues JF, Alpérovitch A. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia: the Three-City cohort study. Neurology. 2007 Nov 13;69(20):1921-30. PubMed.
McGill University Faculty of Medicine
Absence of proof is not proof of absence.
The committee's conclusions reflect the current standards of evidence-based medicine as regards recommendations for interventions, including preventive interventions. By these standards, the field of AD research has not yet produced substantial findings sufficient to warrant such recommendations. That conclusion was never in doubt.
Certainly, the field has attempted to produce such evidence. With one or two exceptions, however, all RCTs have sought either improvement or stabilization of symptomatic AD, including mild cognitive syndromes thought frequently to represent the early symptomatic expression of the disease. The spectacular failures of these trials have brought puzzlement and demoralization, so that we are now forced to reassess our approach to AD therapeutics. In particular, we must consider ways to intervene in the pre-symptomatic stages of AD pathogenesis. From the perspective of symptoms, at least, this is primary prevention.
Until now, our field has done relatively little work on primary prevention. As is well known, primary prevention trials are enormously costly and require many years to complete. Even so, given the looming public health crisis of AD, one may ask why there is reluctance to commit these resources? After all, the U.S. has spent billions on prevention trials for heart disease and cancer. Why not AD?
I suggest that the "elephant in the room" has been our inability—at least until now—to garner preliminary data indicating the probable success of such trials. At present, the best such evidence in AD has come from observational studies, but these have produced inconsistent results, and they are vulnerable to well-known sources of error. Reliance on animal models—an obvious alternative—has proven treacherous, probably because the current models do not resemble human AD sufficiently to provide a realistic test of treatment response.
What we now need, I think, is intensive effort to develop preliminary tests for the "efficacy" of candidate prevention agents in humans. This means finding ways to demonstrate effects of interventions in the pre-symptomatic stages of AD. Fortunately, we are now at the point where we can probe the pre-symptomatic disease process through imaging and biochemical markers. We need to develop the uses of these markers. We need especially to find measures that are sufficiently sensitive and precise to reveal change over time in the pre-symptomatic disease process. Once we have these measures, relatively efficient and short-term experiments can identify which candidate interventions mitigate the progression of pre-symptomatic AD. These same interventions will then become the logical choices for lengthier and more expensive AD prevention trials.
With regard to the big picture, I agree with John Breitner that RCTs have largely focused on treatment of established AD. A few have looked at MCI, but it has been defined at a high threshold, such that the likelihood of progression to dementia in a few years is high—and this typically means that AD pathology is probably already present in most of those who do progress. Primary prevention trials would be optimal, but they require a much longer time period before they will yield informative results, and are also much more expensive.
In the meantime, long-term cohort studies such as Framingham, Nurse's, Health Professionals, Honolulu, etc., may be our best bet, as they offer the possibility of measuring exposure long before symptoms develop. (For brief descriptions of these cohorts, see the AlzRisk AD Epidemiology Database.) This, in turn, rules out the possibility that putative risk factors are actually due to the disease process or part of the disease prodrome, which is a significant concern for cognitive activities, for example.
The call for standardized measures of exposure and outcome is also key. We have made significant gains on the outcome side, though these will need to be enhanced if we move earlier, which we need to do. In contrast, we have a long way yet to go on the exposure side. Convening the principal investigators of major cohort studies to look for common exposure measures might help, although, of course, they will be limited by what they choose to measure years or decades earlier.
In terms of clinical recommendations, one other issue to consider is the "side" consequences of each change. These include other health effects, such as the impact of exercise and dietary changes on cardiovascular health and mortality, and on quality of life. As the old joke goes, "If I do all of this, will I live longer?" "Not necessarily, but it will seem longer."
The consequences of each recommendation may differ from individual to individual, so each person will need to decide based on current evidence and his or her own health history and lifestyle preferences.
University of California, Santa Barbara
The NIH State-of-the-Science Conference has set a threshold of evidence required to make recommendations that goes beyond what would best serve the interests of public health. This may seem paradoxical, because as scientists we demand the highest standards of rigor, and critiquing the panel for setting the bar too high may appear to undermine our strong commitment to the scientific method. However, AD prevention simply is not at the point yet of delivering formal proof as per the dictums of evidence-based medicine. Even so, this panel is likely to influence the thinking of policymakers, who routinely operate in a situation of having to make far-reaching decisions in the absence of definitive proof. Hence, policymakers require guidance that has the public health interest at heart.
The expectation that interventions such as treating hypertension, adopting good nutrition, and exercise require the same standard of proof as demonstrating efficacy and safety of a novel drug represents a further setback for the already arduous task of gaining widespread adherence to healthy behaviors. How would we obtain that standard of proof? The NIH panel does not seriously believe that we will conduct trials in which subjects will be randomized to a control group who do not treat their hypertension, follow a poor diet, or refrain from exercise. And even if we found a clever study design, how would the panel stratify for the multiple variables we encounter in all lifestyle studies? Stratification for a myriad of diets, many levels and types of exercise, a large number of anti-hypertensive treatment regimens, and, most challenging, genotype, would require statistical power that exceeds Earth’s population.
Furthermore, the goals of our communities are to prevent dementia rather than to focus on efficacy in a single cause of dementia, Alzheimer disease. The very large contribution of small vessel disease to the dementia phenotype is completely overlooked when the only outcome measure upon which we base recommendations is strictly Alzheimer’s. Ironically, the panel did note health disparities in the occurrence of dementia, but failed to draw the conclusion that the very groups who suffer most also have a higher incidence of hypertension, poor nutrition, and lack opportunities to exercise.
The panel would have served the nation better had it acknowledged that we have achieved a reasonable level of certainty to recommend risk reduction through safe interventions such as eating well, exercise, and the treatment of hypertension. This is sound both scientifically and from a public health standpoint. Risk reduction does not make the same claim as prevention and allows us to pursue more actively the vital public health efforts directed toward nutrition and obesity in our schools, toward health disparities due to the absence of fresh foods in poor inner city communities, and toward the incontrovertible value of exercise for general well-being. As a scientist, I readily concur that the repertoire of laboratory techniques, which ranges from genetically identical mouse strains to precise quantitative assessments of plaques and tangles and their correlations with advanced imaging procedures, is extraordinarily insightful. But as a physician and member of the community, I think it remains crucial that we not conflate these rigorous approaches, essential as they are to scientific progress, with the best interests of the community now. We have to work for the public good even while we have a less-than-complete dataset.
Make a Comment
To make a comment you must login or register.